Return to color (thing)

The Visible Spectrum

The human [eye] can perceive [electromagnetic wave|electromagnetic waves] with [wavelength|wavelengths] from approximately 390[nanometer|nm] (violet) to 750[nanometer|nm] (red). The visible wavelengths of light are collectively referred to as the [visible spectrum], and constitue a small part of the total [electromagnetic spectrum]. From longest to shortest wavelength, the colors are: [red], [orange], [yellow], [green], [blue], and [violet]. A chart of the visible spectrum is just a cross section of rainbow that labels the colors with what wavelength of light they produce (see [http://www.photo.net/photo/edscott/vis00010.htm]). Other animals can see different frequency ranges, but otherwise their visible spectrum is fundamentally no different from ours.

The Color Wheel

You can probably remember seeing a [color wheel] somewhere back in elementary school - that picture with the primary colors and intermediates you would get if you mixed them together? Technically, if yours had [red], [yellow], and [blue] (or [magenta], [yellow], and [cyan]) as the [primary colors], it was demonstrating subtractive colors - the kind of colors you get by mixing paints together. Light is additive, but the concept is the same; for light, the primary colors are red, green, and blue, and the intermediates are brighter than the primaries because there is more total light present in them. Check out [http://home.wanadoo.nl/paulschils/06.00.html] for a nice picture of an additive color wheel.

The [visible spectrum] is laid out in a line, but the [color wheel] is circular. You can get a fairly good idea of their relationship if you imagine bending the visible spectrum into a circle. The color wheel is useful for observing multiple [wavelength|wavelengths] of [light] simultaneously (what do I get if I mix red and blue?). The [color wheel] is not representative of the real world - only our [perception] of it. For many species, our color wheel would be meaningless.

Primary Colors

The [primary colors] are the basis for the color wheel. The whole point of the wheel is that all the other colors on the wheel are made up of varying amounts of just those three colors. However, the primary colors are entirely a product of our [perception] of [light] - they are meaningless outside of our eyes. Each [primary colors|primary color] of light ([red], [green], and [blue]) corresponds to a type of cone in our eyes suited to observe that particular wavelength of light. Red cones are best at 564[nanometer|nm] wavelengths, green cones at 534[nanometer|nm], and blue cones at 420[nanometer|nm]. Upon seeing red light, the "red" cones are excited and send an appropriate signal to the brain. Yellow light (560[nanometer|nm]) is between red and green in wavelength, and will excite both red and green cones - the brain interprets the combined red-green signal as yellow. Your brain cannot tell the difference between a pure single-frequency yellow light, and a combination green/red light that triggers the green and red cones in the same proportion and amplitude.

Computer monitors take advantage of this fact. If you look at a monitor with a magnifying glass, you will see tiny clusters of three dots; red, green, and blue. By adjusting the [brightness] of each dot, our [eye|eyes] can be fooled into thinking they are seeing any color of the [visible spectrum] (and even a few that aren't in the spectrum - keep reading).

So if the [primary colors] are only a function of the cones in our [eye|eyes], could animals have different primary colors? Yes, and in fact they do. The red-sensitive cone is a fairly recent [evolution] in primates, and many mammals (including dogs and cats) do not have red sensing cones in their eyes. [bee|Bees], on the other hand, have extra primary colors, including one in the [ultraviolet]! The number of possible colors grows exponentially with the number of primary colors available because each color is perceived as a combination of the primaries.

What About Violet?

It is easy to see how mixing red (650[nanometer|nm]) and green (535[nanometer|nm]) light might make you see yellow (560nm) light, because yellow is somewhere in between red and green. A [wavelength] between red and green will trigger both the red and green cones, and can be mimicked by separately triggering both. Similarly, any [frequency] of [light] between green and blue can be recreated in our eyes by using a combination of pure green and blue light (remember that the light does not actually combine to form an intermediate [frequency], it just looks that way to our eyes). But why would combining red light (700[nanometer|nm]) with blue light (420[nanometer|nm]) make our eyes perceive violet (390[nanometer|nm])?

Each of the three types of [cone|cones] in our eyes is tuned to a particular wavelength of light (Red: 564[nanometer|nm], Green: 534[nanometer|nm], Blue: 420[nanometer|nm]). These are the wavelengths of light each of these cones responds to most strongly, but they are not the only wavelengths they respond to. Green and blue cones have fairly centralized responses around their associated wavelength, but red cones actually respond faintly to wavelengths much shorter than normal red light. The red response declines steadily from 564[nanometer|nm] to about 500[nanometer|nm], but then remains constant and even rises slightly below 450[nanometer|nm]. This means that although blue light primarily triggers the blue cones, it also excites the red ones slightly.

When we see the blue [light], we get a lot of response from the blue [cone|cones] and a slight response from the red ones. Heading toward [violet], the red response remains slight but steady, while the blue response falls off. In this way, the wavelengths below blue make our eyes see proportionately more red, even though the absolute value of red cone response is remaining the same! Green and blue cone response is much more restricted to their respective wavelengths; under violet light, green cones will respond very slightly, but not enough to be noticeable compared to the blue or red cone response.

It is impossible to see "pure" green or blue, because green and blue light will always trigger a slight red cone response. If the [optical nerves] were directly [stimulate|stimulated], it might be possible to see a color more green than is physically possible; under ordinary circumstances all green light will trigger some red cone response in addition to the main green cone one.

Now what if you have light composed of a lot of red with a little bit of blue - a color like [magenta]. This is not a color that can be achieved with a single [wavelength] of light! Take a look at the [visible spectrum] ([http://www.photo.net/photo/edscott/vis00010.htm]) - magenta isn't there. Any color between violet and red on the color wheel actually requires two wavelengths of light to create. Of course, white light (when all three types of [cone|cones] are active) requires multiple wavelengths of [light] as well.

Also in the [visible spectrum], you will notice that at shorter wavelengths than blue the colors turn more and more violet until we can't see them. But once you reach red, our vision just stays red - once our red cones are active and the blue and green cones are not, we have no way of distinguishing 675[nanometer|nm] from 700[nanometer|nm] red. The longer wavelengths will seem [dark|darker] the further they get from the red cone's main frequency, but to us this is indistinguishable from just dim [light|lighting].

References:

[http://madsci.wustl.edu/posts/archives/mar2001/985572799.Ph.r.html]

[http://www.photo.net/photo/edscott/vis00010.htm]
Existing:


Non-Existing: